U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

NCBI Bookshelf. A service of the National Library of Medicine, National Institutes of Health.

National Research Council (US) Committee on Recognition and Alleviation of Pain in Laboratory Animals. Recognition and Alleviation of Pain in Laboratory Animals. Washington (DC): National Academies Press (US); 2009.

Cover of Recognition and Alleviation of Pain in Laboratory Animals

Recognition and Alleviation of Pain in Laboratory Animals.

Show details

3Recognition and Assessment of Pain

This chapter begins with a presentation of the clinical signs and behaviors that veterinarians use to recognize animals in pain. It then provides a review of methods for pain assessment, with a focus on techniques for specific laboratory animal species. It concludes with species-specific clinical signs and behavioral responses to pain.

INTRODUCTION

Recognizing pain and assessing its intensity are both essential for its effective management. If pain is not recognized, then it is unlikely to be treated; failure to appreciate the intensity of pain will hamper the selection of an appropriately potent analgesic, raise doubts about the effectiveness of the administered dose, and result in less than optimal treatment. In humans, self-report of pain is the “gold standard” by which other assessment techniques may be judged, although there are limitations and biases even when using this approach (see Chapter 1). For animals, as for humans who cannot self-report (e.g., the very young and those with cognitive impairment; Ranger et al. 2007; Zwakhalen et al. 2006), other assessment tools are necessary.

Since the publication of the first edition of this report (NRC 1992), there have been considerable advances in scientists’ understanding of animal pain and numerous attempts to develop methods of assessing pain. Yet few validated assessment techniques are available. In most circumstances pain is assessed based on an animal’s clinical appearance and overall behavior. Although this approach can be unreliable, it is usually effective in detecting severe pain in many species. It is also effective when pain is localized to one limb (causing lameness) or to a specific body area (resulting in a marked behavioral response if that area is palpated).

The ability to assess pain in laboratory animals will improve with the development of validated, objective schemes for particular species and types of procedures. Some schemes of this type are in development, while others (e.g., assessment of postsurgical pain in dogs [Morton et al. 2005] or of pain after abdominal surgery in rats [Roughan and Flecknell 2001, 2003]) have reached the point that they can be used to assess pain in the particular species in a variety of situations. It is also possible that some of the behaviors noted may occur in other species: contraction of the abdominal muscles following abdominal surgery is observed in rats and has also been reported in mice (Wright-Williams et al. 2007) and rabbits (Leach et al. 2009). Regardless of the assessment technique, however, it is important that it be done by a team that includes researchers, veterinarians, and animal care staff.

PAIN RECOGNITION: CLINICAL SIGNS AND BEHAVIOR

There are no generally accepted objective criteria for assessing the degree of pain that an animal is experiencing. Species vary widely in their response to pain, and often animals of the same species show different responses to different types of pain. Box 3-1 presents a basic algorithm for pain assessment that may serve until the development of species-specific pain assessment methods. A team approach and cooperative spirit among all interested parties—researchers, veterinarians, and animal care staff—will benefit the welfare of the animal in pain.

Box Icon

BOX 3-1

Pain Assessment Protocol. The following approach can be helpful for assessing pain in particular animal models: Prepare a checklist of the examinations to be undertaken, allow space for a general comment, and perhaps include an overall assessment tool (more...)

It is important that clinical evaluations and assessment protocols be carried out by individuals with a detailed knowledge of the normal and abnormal behavior and appearance of the species concerned. Further, the effects of the observer on the behavior of the animal should be considered; for example, some species, such as rabbits and guinea pigs, may remain immobile, especially if the observer is an unfamiliar person. In these cases, it may be necessary to observe the animal via a camera or viewing panel. When assessing behavioral changes, it is often helpful to have a checklist that may incorporate a grading scheme (see the scoring system developed by Morton and Griffiths in 1985). However, because different individuals often fail to agree on the score that should be assigned (Beynen et al. 1987) it may be simpler to note the presence or absence of a specific clinical sign. Changes in successive observations could indicate an improvement or deterioration in the animal’s condition. Although many observations will not be specific indicators of pain, a structured examination is always helpful in monitoring an animal’s progress during a study. Table 3-1 presents a number of behavioral signs usually associated with pain.

TABLE 3-1. Behavioral Signs of Persistent Pain.

TABLE 3-1

Behavioral Signs of Persistent Pain.

Animals in pain reduce their overall level of activity, as observed, for example, in mice following surgery (Clark et al. 2004; Karas 2002; Wright-Williams et al. 2007). It has been suggested that changes in heart rate, respiratory rate, and blood pressure can be used to assess pain, but these clinical parameters are often unreliable or nonspecific (e.g., similar changes may be observed in stressed or distressed animals; NRC 2008). Consistent changes in these parameters in animals expected to be in pain have not been demonstrated (Cambridge et al. 2000; Holton et al. 1998; Price et al. 2003). Given the range of factors (e.g., fear, excitement) that can alter heart and respiratory rate, this is not surprising, as even handling can cause major changes in heart rate, respiratory rate, and blood pressure. Recently, however, more sophisticated analysis of heart rate variability has been of value as an adjunct to pain assessment (Arras et al. 2007; Rietmann et al. 2004).

PAIN ASSESSMENT METHODS

As discussed above, methods for assessing pain in laboratory animals remain highly subjective and are based largely on preconceived ideas about the appearance and behavior of animals in response to pain. Attempts to apply the Morton and Griffiths (1985) scoring scheme were largely unsuccessful (Beynen et al. 1987), primarily because the variables selected for inclusion were not fully identified and the ratings (0–3) not sufficiently well characterized (this scheme has proven much more successful in the development of humane endpoints for studies that may cause distress rather than pain; NRC 2008).

In addition to the lack of known effective pain assessment methods, it is not uncommon for a study to include the administration of an analgesic without any attempt to evaluate its effectiveness. For example, a recent survey of pain control in laboratory animals in the United Kingdom found that, although all the institutions in the survey used analgesics, almost none used methods of pain assessment to confirm that the treatment was effective (Hawkins 2002).

Behavioral Changes

Objective measures likely to indicate pain include changes in general locomotor activity (e.g., guarding a specific area or avoiding weight-bearing on an injured limb; Duncan et al. 1991; Flecknell and Liles 1991; Malavasi et al. 2006) and in food and water intake and body weight (Liles and Flecknell 1992, 1993a,b). These measures are also useful to assess analgesic drug efficacy, although because they are retrospective they cannot be used to modify analgesic therapy for a particular animal. They are, however, effective as a simple measure of postoperative recovery and as a means of adjusting future analgesic regimens for similar animals undergoing similar surgical procedures.

Influences of Analgesics on Behavior

The use of analgesics warrants certain cautions. Some analgesics, notably opioids, cause marked behavioral changes in healthy, pain-free animals, which can confound attempts to assess pain (Roughan and Flecknell 2000). Buprenorphine stimulates activity in normal mice (Cowan et al. 1977; Hayes et al. 2000), so behavioral changes after the use of this drug during surgery could be due to the provision of effective pain relief or a nonspecific drug effect. In contrast, NSAIDs have only very minor effects on behavior in healthy, pain-free animals, so this problem is not significant with the use of these analgesics (Roughan and Flecknell 2001; Wright-Williams et al. 2007).

Further, significant behavioral signs of postsurgical pain in rodents may persist only 6 to 8 hours after some procedures (Roughan and Flecknell 2004), so these results may be due to administration of analgesics to animals that were not experiencing pain. In these circumstances side effects such as sedation or nausea may be of much greater significance. For more information on other behavioral measures readers are referred to Chapter 1, especially Box 1-4.

Moreover, the influence of analgesics on body weight following surgery is not always easy to interpret. In some studies, after an initial presumed beneficial effect, animals that had undergone surgery and not received postoperative analgesics gained more weight over a 2- to 3-day period than their counterparts under an analgesic regime (Sharp et al. 2003).

Behavioral Assessment Studies in Rats, Mice, and Rabbits

Investigators have described specific behavioral changes following abdominal surgery and ureteral calculosis in rats (Giamberardino et al. 1995; Gonzalez et al. 2000; Roughan and Flecknell 2000) and these behaviors have been incorporated in a practicable pain assessment tool for use in laboratory rats after abdominal surgery (Roughan and Flecknell 2002). During the initial development of the scheme, rat behavior was evaluated both before and after a midline laparotomy with appropriate untreated and anesthetic and analgesic controls.

An initial study using buprenorphine as the analgesic was inconclusive because of the marked effects of this opioid on normal behavior (Roughan and Flecknell 2001). A subsequent study using carprofen and ketoprofen successfully identified behaviors that differentiated rats that had (1) undergone surgery from those that had simply been anesthetized and (2) received analgesics after surgery from those that had not. These studies required detailed analysis of considerable periods of videotaped behavior including filming at night under red light. The utility of these behaviors was further demonstrated in rats undergoing surgery as part of an unrelated research project that entailed placing the animals in an observation cage for a 15-minute period and assessing the frequency of the pain-related behaviors. Again, it was possible to differentiate animals receiving analgesics from untreated controls, and to demonstrate a dose-related effect of the NSAID meloxicam (Roughan and Flecknell 2003).

When experienced staff (animal technicians, research workers, and veterinarians) first viewed selected video recordings from these animals, they were unable to correctly identify the treatment groups. However, after watching a short recording illustrating the key behaviors, their ability to identify animals that had or had not received analgesics greatly improved (Roughan and Flecknell 2006). These studies suggest that key behaviors can be identified and used to score pain following one type of surgical procedure in rats. In addition, the studies underscore the importance of proper training of even experienced personnel with the introducton of new techniques. It is not yet clear whether behavioral changes in rats after various surgical procedures will differ greatly in type or will be drawn from a common group of abnormal, pain-related behaviors.

Recent studies in mice have indicated that they experience similar pain-related changes in behavior after abdominal surgery (Wright-Williams et al. 2007) and that these behaviors might form the basis of a murine pain scoring scheme. However, the rapid movement of mice makes observations less reliable. In addition, the effects of the analgesics used in these studies were less predictable than in rats as were the effects of opioids, which, as mentioned above, affect behavior in normal animals. These studies also found a major difference in the frequency of pain-related behaviors in the two different strains of mice used (C3He and C57Bl6). Other studies (e.g., Karas 2002) have shown changes in the frequency of normal activity in mice after surgery, and it may be possible to develop a scoring system based on a combination of changes in abnormal and normal activity.

In some instances, changes in a specific locomotor pattern, or gait, can be assessed objectively using a variety of techniques (Gabriel et al. 2007). Force plates and other means of assessing limb use and gait have been used to evaluate the severity of arthritis in laboratory and companion animals as well as the efficacy of analgesic therapy (Gabriel et al. 2007; Hazewinkel et al. 2008). The linking of clinical signs to behavioral alterations after administration of an analgesic facilitates pain assessment.

A small number of studies have attempted to assess postsurgical pain in rabbits. Initial attempts to develop a behavior-based scheme failed because of the animals’ reaction to the presence of an observer (Roughan and Flecknell 2004), and a similar study produced inconclusive results (Parga 2002). More recently, a detailed assessment of behavior before and after surgery, using remotely operated cameras, revealed clearly identifiable abnormal behaviors as well as changes in the frequency of normal behaviors. The effects of analgesics were limited. Further work is required before clear recommendations can be made about the usefulness of these behaviors (Leach et al. 2009).

A problem with all of these behavior-based schemes is that in many instances the animals studied were anesthetized with regimens (e.g., isoflurane or sevoflurane) that resulted in rapid recovery of consciousness. When recovery is delayed, or is associated with prolonged sedation, animals may fail to express pain behavior and scoring may therefore not be reliable. The scoring system may also be influenced by other factors, such as the animals’ fear and apprehension, or unexpected variations in behavior between different strains (Wright-Williams et al. 2007). Nevertheless, detailed behavioral observations are a step forward in developing a practical and useful pain scoring system for use after surgery in laboratory animals. What is not yet known is whether similar systems can be used to develop a means of identifying and quantifying other types of pain in animals, including chronic pain.

Developing Objective Pain Assessment Tools: Companion Animals

Initial methods for scoring pain in companion animals were largely subjective and seriously flawed. Some studies, however, demonstrated that behavioral assessments could be used to evaluate the effects of surgery and analgesia (e.g., the use of visual analogue scores to assess pain following ovariohysterectomy in dogs [Lascelles et al. 1997] and cats [Slingsby and Waterman-Pearson 1998]). Additional scoring schemes for use in dogs have since been developed (Firth and Haldane 1999; Holton et al. 2001), and numerous studies use VAS, numerical rating systems, simple descriptive scores, or a mix of the three approaches (Brodbelt et al. 1997; Mathews et al. 2001). These different approaches highlight the problems involved in developing pain assessment schemes (Holton et al. 1998); for example,

  • the assessment criteria are frequently highly subjective,
  • the study designs do not include untreated (surgery and no analgesia) controls,
  • the study designs do not include anesthesia and analgesia (and no surgery) control groups, and/or
  • only a single dosage is assessed rather than a range of doses.

Firth and Haldane (1999) videotaped dogs before and after surgery and, after making detailed observations, identified behaviors that were probable indicators of pain. In common with other behavior-based scoring schemes, they hypothesized that behaviors that appeared only after surgery, or that increased or decreased greatly after surgery, could be pain-related. If administration of an analgesic normalized these behavioral changes, this provided additional evidence that the changes were due to pain. The scheme set out by Firth and Haldane has been developed further and recommended as a tool suitable for clinical use (Gaynor and Muir 2002).

Holton and colleagues (2001) adopted a different approach. This group sought to identify descriptors of pain by consulting with experienced small animal clinicians, and then used sophisticated analytical techniques to reduce these descriptors to a set of words or phrases that could be developed into a multidimensional pain scale. Unfortunately, validation in a placebo-controlled, blinded study has yet to be completed.

It is important to note that the development of a pain score essentially based on the opinion of clinician experts is almost certain to result in a self-fulfilling scheme that will detect pain and predict which animals will receive additional analgesics, since it will be used by clinicians whose opinion shaped its development. Because this is a common problem in pain scoring of both animals and humans, these schemes should be developed further and validated through randomized, blind, placebo-controlled trials.

Placebo-controlled studies in animals, however, pose significant ethical and practical difficulties. Because most schemes include some behavioral assessments, and because anesthetics and analgesics, notably opioids, can markedly change behavior in normal, pain-free animals, lack of appropriate controls (i.e., postprocedural animals that receive no anesthetic or analgesic) can make the results highly questionable. The inclusion, however, of such control groups may cause significant ethical dilemmas to researchers that undertake pain assessment studies, most of which are carried out in veterinary schools. Deliberately withholding analgesics in circumstances believed likely to result in pain may be considered unacceptable by students who learn that animals experience pain and should receive analgesics. Studies of pain with human participants require an intervention analgesia protocol so that subjects assessed as experiencing pain above a predetermined level are removed from the study and given an analgesic. This approach has been used in a number of veterinary clinical studies (Grisneaux et al. 1999; Lascelles et al. 1995).

Measurement of Nociceptive Responses

A wide variety of methods for measuring nociceptive response apply to either momentary or more longer-lasting noxious stimuli for research purposes (Hogan 2002; Le Bars et al. 2001).1 Although these have limited application for assessing pain in other situations (e.g., after surgery), they do provide insight into potential pain-related behaviors and can help predict effective analgesic drug dose rates. Techniques that measure momentary nociceptive responses involve the application of a brief noxious stimulus followed by quantification of the animal’s response. Administration of analgesics usually modifies this response, for example by prolonging the latency of withdrawal of a limb or tail from the noxious stimulus. In addition to the use of such techniques in small laboratory animals, they have been applied to studies in larger species to assess analgesic efficacy and detect the occurrence of hyperalgesia after injury (Dixon et al. 2002; KuKanich et al. 2005; Ley and Waterman 1996; Pypendop et al. 2006; Slingsby et al. 2001; Veissier et al. 2000; Welsh and Nolan 1995).

Although primarily used as a means of screening for potential analgesics in drug discovery programs, the results of nociception measurement have been used to estimate dose rates of analgesics for clinical use in both large and small animals. Such extrapolations, however, must be made with caution. In one study, estimates of appropriate doses of buprenorphine based on tail flick tests resulted in a recommended dose of 0.5 mg/kg in rats (Flecknell 1984), 10 times higher than that proven to be effective in postoperative pain scoring systems (Roughan and Flecknell 2004). Since high doses of this agent can have undesirable side effects, it is important to approach these extrapolations very carefully.

Although the results of these tests may not predict clinical efficacy, they do illustrate the very wide variation in response among different strains of rodents (Mogil et al. 1999; Morgan et al. 1999) and thus reinforce the importance of developing pain scoring schemes. If appropriate schemes cannot be used, then dose rates are probably best estimated based on the results of inflammatory pain models such as the late-phase formalin test (Roughan and Flecknell 2002; Appendix A provides details).

Biological Markers of Nociceptor Activation

Although biomarkers of nociceptor activation can be used only as research tools, they can indicate whether a particular procedure could cause pain. For example, the early gene product c-fos (Coggeshall 2005) has been used as a marker of nociceptor activity in a number of species (Lykkegaard et al. 2005; Svendsen et al. 2007). Such assessments are possible only within a short time after the animal is euthanized and so are not suitable for routine clinical use.

As discussed in Chapter 2, nociceptor activation and some of the other peripheral and central changes associated with pain and tissue damage result in alterations of sensory thresholds, notably hyperalgesia and allodynia (the perception of previously nonnoxious stimuli as noxious). These changes have been used as indicators of both nociceptor activity and the efficacy of analgesic therapy in both laboratory and clinical studies (Lascelles et al. 1997; Whiteside et al. 2004). Although these methods essentially measure peripheral changes, it is reasonable to assume that in conscious animals such changes indicate that pain has been experienced and may still be present.

Brain Activity Imaging

Recent imaging studies have demonstrated that exposure to noxious stimuli activates a range of cortical and subcortical areas—both primary somatosensory cortex and areas associated with the affective component of pain in humans (Hess et al. 2007). Although such activation does not demonstrate awareness of pain in animals, it clearly indicates activation of the cortical areas considered necessary for the affective component of pain (see also Box 1-3). The use of imaging offers a novel approach for detecting central processing of nociceptive information in animals and may enable a more objective assessment of the potential for particular procedures or conditions to cause pain.

PAIN ASSESSMENT: SPECIES-SPECIFIC CLINICAL SIGNS

There is a remarkable lack of validated behavioral signs of pain in many species (Viñuela-Fernández et al. 2007). The following sections present a number of species-specific clinical manifestations based on expert clinical opinion and best practices. Although the signs described typically accompany or indicate pain, many are not specific to pain and may occur as general signs of ill health or as responses to stress or distress (readers are encouraged to consult the ethograms and tables with species-specific clinical signs indicating pain, distress, or discomfort in the appendix of the 2008 NRC report Recognition and Alleviation of Distress in Laboratory Animals).

Nonhuman Primates

Nonhuman primates show remarkably little reaction to surgical procedures or to injury, especially in the presence of humans, and might look well until they are gravely ill or in severe pain. Viewing an animal from a distance or by video can aid in detecting subtle clinical changes. A nonhuman primate that appears sick is likely to be critically ill and might require rapid attention.

A nonhuman primate in pain has a general appearance of misery and dejection. It might huddle in a crouched posture with its arms across its chest and its head forward with a “sad” facial expression or a grimace and glassy eyes. It might moan or scream,2 avoid its companions, and stop grooming. A monkey in pain can also attract altered attention from its cagemates, varying from a lack of social grooming to attack. The animal may show acute abdominal pain through facial contortions, clenching of teeth, restlessness, and shaking accompanied by grunts and moans. Head pain may be manifest by head pressing against the enclosure surface. Self-directed injurious behavior may be a sign of more intense pain. Primates in pain usually refuse food and water. If an animal is well socialized or trained to perform tasks as part of a research protocol, changes in response to familiar personnel or in willingness to cooperate may indicate pain.

Dogs

Dogs in pain generally appear less alert and quieter than normal although small breeds are generally more reactive to environmental changes than large dogs. Dogs in pain may move stiffly or be unwilling to move, and if in severe pain may lie still or adopt an abnormal posture to minimize discomfort. In less severe pain, dogs can appear restless and more alert. Other apparent potential changes include inappetence, shivering, and increased respiration with panting. Dogs in pain may bite, scratch, or guard painful regions and if handled may be unusually apprehensive or aggressive. Their response to a familiar handler may be different; for example, a dog in pain may fail to wag its tail or may shrink away. Incessant licking is sometimes associated with localized pain. Pain in one limb usually results in limping or holding up of the affected limb with no attempt to use it. Spontaneous barking is unlikely; dogs are more likely to whimper or howl, especially if unattended, and may growl without apparent provocation. However, lack of vocalization or excessive vocalization is not a reliable indicator of pain.

Cats

With cats, which are less noticeably reactive to environmental changes than dogs, a general lack of well-being is an important indication of pain. A cat in pain is generally quiet and has an apprehensive facial expression (e.g., its forehead may appear creased). The animal may cry, yowl, growl, or hiss if approached or made to move. It tends to hide or to separate itself from other cats. Its posture becomes stiff and abnormal, varying with the site of pain. If the pain is in the head or ears, the animal might tilt its head toward the affected side. A cat with generalized pain in both the thorax and abdomen may crouch or hunch. If the pain is only thoracic, the head, neck, and body might be extended. A cat with abdominal or back pain might stand or lie on its side with its back arched or walk with a stilted gait. Incessant licking is sometimes associated with localized pain. Pain in one limb usually results in limping or holding up of the affected limb with no attempt to use it. Cats in severe or chronic pain look ungroomed and behave markedly differently from normal. Touching or palpation of a painful area might produce an immediate violent reaction and an attempt to escape. A reduction in food and water intake may be an indicator of pain.

Laboratory Rodents

Rats and mice are the two rodent species most widely used in research generally and in pain-related studies specifically, so it is important that researchers and institutional animal care and use committees recognize when these animals are in pain (for additional information see Chapter 1: Boxes 1-3 and 1-4, Chapter 4, and Appendix A). Rats and mice in acute pain may vocalize and become unusually aggressive when handled. Because rodents also vocalize at ultrasonic frequencies inaudible to humans, the absence of audible vocalization does not necessarily signify the absence of acute pain. Inappetence or a change in feeding activity may become evident; for example, the animals may eat bedding or their offspring. If they are housed with others, the normal group behavior or grooming might change. Rodents in pain may separate from their cagemates and attempt to hide, or they may no longer exhibit nest-building behavior. In rats, porphyrin secretion (“red tears”) may appear around the eyes and nose, although this is a general response to stress of any kind.

Normal guinea pigs stampede and squeal when startled, when attempts are made to handle them, or when strangers are in the room, but sick guinea pigs and those in pain usually remain quiet. However, because a normal guinea pig’s initial response to the presence of an observer is also to remain immobile, assessing signs of pain can be extremely difficult. Guinea pigs in pain reduce their food and water consumption and may become anorexic. As with rabbits, this behavior can exacerbate the ileus (i.e., gut stasis) that may occur following surgery and can result in a fatal enterotoxemia.

There is virtually no information about signs of pain in hamsters and gerbils, although it is assumed that when in pain they, like rats and mice, will show decreased activity, piloerection, and an ungroomed appearance. As with other species they may adopt an abnormal posture, which may be particularly obvious when moving. Respiration may change.

Rabbits

Rabbits in pain may appear apprehensive, anxious, dull, or inactive, assume a hunched appearance, attempt to hide, and squeal or cry. But sometimes they show aggressive behavior with increased activity and excessive scratching and licking. Reactions to handling are exaggerated, and acute pain might result in vocalization. With abdominal pain, they may show back arching when moving, contraction of the abdominal muscles, and pressing of the abdomen to the ground. Although teeth-grinding has been identified as an indicator of pain, it is not a reliable behavioral sign and studies to support its usefulness as a pain indicator have not yet been done. The respiratory rate of the animals may increase and they may eat and drink less. As with rodents, surgery in rabbits can result in ileus and this, coupled with pain-associated inappetence, can lead to the development of a fatal enterotoxemia. As with other species, a general lack of grooming may be associated with pain.

Ungulates

The greatest progress in developing objective behavior-based methods of assessing the response to pain and injury has been in farm animals. Behavioral and endocrine indicators of pain in lambs, cattle, and pigs have been established by a number of research groups (Hay et al. 2003; Lester et al. 1996; Mellor and Stafford 2000; Molony et al. 2002; Noonan et al. 1994) and vocalization patterns in piglets have been analyzed as potential indicators of pain (Puppe et al. 2005; Weary et al. 1998). These measures have been developed largely to aid in the evaluation of the welfare benefits of modifying standard agricultural practices such as tail docking, castration, and dehorning. It has been repeatedly demonstrated that use of local anesthetics, either alone or in conjunction with modifications to the techniques commonly used, can reduce pain-related behaviors in lambs and cattle (Mellor and Stafford 2000). These studies not only allowed ranking of the degree of pain caused by different procedures but also highlighted some of the problems associated with the use of behavioral signs as indicators of pain. For example, lambs castrated using a rubber ring to constrict the neck of the scrotum show a series of very easily identified abnormal behaviors associated with pain. Lambs castrated surgically without anesthesia remain largely immobile for prolonged periods but the endocrine stress response produced by this method is even greater than that produced by rubber ring occlusion (Lester et al. 1991). Because the types of behaviors observed in lambs undergoing these different procedures varied, it was not possible to use behavior alone to rank the degree of pain. However, the behavioral responses could be used to compare methods of reducing the pain associated with each procedure (Molony et al. 2002).

Horses

Horses in acute pain show reluctance to be handled and other varied responses (Ashley et al. 2005; Driessen and Zarucco 2007): periods of restlessness, interrupted feeding with food held in the mouth uneaten, anxious appearance with dilated pupils and glassy eyes, increased respiration and pulse rate with flared nostrils, profuse sweating, and a rigid stance. Horses in pain also grind their teeth, switch their tails, or play with their water bucket. For animals in prolonged pain, behavior may change from restlessness to depression with head lowered. In pain associated with skeletal damage, there is reluctance to move; the animal may hold its limbs in unusual positions (e.g., it may stand “parked” with the weight on the hind feet and one front foot “pointed” ahead of the other), and the head and neck in a fixed position. Horses with abdominal or thoracic pain may look at, bite, or kick their abdomen; get up and lie down frequently; walk in circles; stand “parked” with elbows adducted; and sweat, roll, and injure themselves as a result of these activities, with bruising especially around the eyes.

Cattle

Cattle in pain often appear dull and depressed, hold their heads low, and show little interest in their surroundings. Their overall activity may be reduced (Hudson et al. 2008). Other observable changes include inappetence, weight loss, grunting, grinding of teeth, and, in milking cows, decreased milk yield (Hernandez et al. 2002, 2005). Severe pain often results in rapid, shallow respiration. On handling, the animals may react violently or adopt a rigid posture to immobilize the painful region. Localized pain may be associated with persistent licking or kicking at the offending area and, when the pain is severe, bellowing. Generally, signs of abdominal pain are similar to those in horses, but less marked. Rigid posture can lead to a lack of grooming because of an unwillingness to turn the neck. With acute abdominal conditions, such as intestinal strangulation, cattle adopt a characteristic stance with one hind foot placed directly in front of the other.

The behavior of calves after dehorning and castration without anesthesia has been described in detail (Molony et al. 1995; Stafford and Mellor 2005) and includes decreased rumination and feeding and an increased incidence of ear flicking, tail flicking, and head shaking. After castration using a rubber ring, calves showed restlessness, foot stamping/kicking, stretching, and adjustments of posture (“easing quarter”); in contrast, after crushing (Burdizzo) or surgical castration the most marked behavioral change was “statue” standing (Molony et al. 1995).

Sheep and Goats

Signs of pain in sheep and goats are generally similar to those in cattle, but sheep, in particular, tolerate severe injury without overt signs of pain or distress. There is a general reluctance to move, coupled with changes in posture, movement, and facial expression. Pain can also cause cessation of rumination, eating, and drinking, and increased curling of the lips; but, as in other species, these are not reliable indicators of pain. Goats are more likely than cattle to vocalize in response to pain. They may also grind their teeth, have rapid and shallow breathing, change posture frequently, and appear agitated (stamping their feet). Dairy goats quickly decrease production and lose body weight and general body condition. After castration or tail docking, lambs show very characteristic signs of pain by standing and lying repeatedly, wagging their tails, occasionally bleating, and displaying neck extension, dorsal lip curling, kicking, rolling, and hyperventilation (Molony et al. 2002).

Pigs

Pigs in pain might show changes in their overall demeanor, social behavior, gait, and posture as well as an absence of bed making. They may become apathetic and unwilling to move and may hide in bedding if possible. Pigs normally squeal and attempt to escape when handled, and pain can accentuate these reactions or cause adults to become aggressive. Squealing is also characteristic when painful areas are palpated. More moderate pain may simply reduce activity levels and make the animal less responsive to familiar handlers and reluctant to feed or drink (Harvey-Clark et al. 2000; Malavasi et al. 2006).

Birds and Poultry

Birds in pain show escape reactions, vocalization, and excessive movement. Small species struggle less and emit fewer distress calls than large species. Head movements increase in extent and frequency. There may also be an increase in heart and respiratory rates. Birds in chronic pain may exhibit a passive immobility characterized by a crouched posture with closed or partially closed eyes and head drawn toward the body. They may also become inappetent and inactive with a drooping, miserable appearance, holding their wings flat against the body and their neck retracted. There may be reduced perching or birds may remain at the bottom of the cage. When a bird is handled, its escape reaction may be replaced by immobility. Birds with limb pain avoid use of the affected limb and refrain from extension.

Reptiles

Acute pain in reptiles is characterized by flinching and muscle contractions. There might be aversive movements away from the unpleasant stimulus and attempts to bite. Chronic and persistent pain may be associated with inappetence, lethargy, and weight loss, although it is difficult to associate any of these signs of lack of well-being specifically with pain.

Fish

It is difficult to determine the nature of the response to pain in fish or whether their experience is similar to that observed in mammals (ILAR 2009; Rose 2002; Sneddon 2006; see Chapter 1). Although there have been few species-specific studies, there is evidence that fish exhibit a pronounced initial response to injuries or to contact with nociceptive stimuli or chemical algesics (Sneddon 2003; Sneddon et al. 2003a,b; Reilly et al. 2008; Ashley et al. 2009) but their response to chronic stimuli has not been characterized. Generally, fish react to noxious stimuli (such as puncture with a hypodermic needle) with strong muscular movements, and when exposed to a noxious environment (such as an acidic solution) show abnormal swimming behavior, attempts to jump from the water, and more rapid opercular movements. Such effects indicate some, perhaps considerable, distress, but it is not possible to state unequivocally that it is pain-induced distress.

Recent research has identified nociceptors in fish (Ashley et al. 2006, 2007; Sneddon 2002; Sneddon et al. 2003a) that are physiologically similar to mammalian nociceptors. In vivo administration of a noxious stimulus resulted in aberrant behaviors (rocking on the substrate and rubbing of the affected area) and adverse changes in physiology in rainbow trout over a period of 3 to 6 hours (Sneddon et al. 2003a,b); morphine reduced the incidence of these behaviors (Sneddon 2003; Sneddon et al. 2003b). Recent research has also shown that, after a one-time subcutaneous injection of 1% acetic acid to the lower and upper frontal lip, trout do not show appropriate neophobic or antipredator behaviors when compared to behavioral impairments associated with pain (Ashley et al. 2009; Sneddon et al. 2003b). Goldfish given electric shock display agitated swimming behavior but the threshold for this response increases if morphine is injected, while naloxone blocks the morphine effect (Jansen and Greene 1970). Work by Ehrensing and colleagues (1982) showed that the endogenous opioid antagonist MIF-1 downregulates sensitivity to opioids in goldfish, which then do not show an escape response to electric shock.

Studies have shown that goldfish are able to learn to avoid noxious, potentially painful stimuli such as electric shock (Portavella et al. 2002, 2004). Learned avoidance of a stimulus associated with a noxious experience has also been observed in other fish species including common carp and pike (Esox lucius), which avoided hooks in angling trials (Beukema 1970a,b; Overmier and Hollis 1983, 1990).

Amphibians

Amphibian species such as anurans (frogs and toads) and urodeles (salamanders) are commonly used in laboratory animal research settings (Schaeffer 1997), but there is no objective means to assess the presence and severity of pain in amphibians, especially since they do not exhibit any facial expression (Hadfield and Whitaker 2005). Some exotic animal clinicians use nonspecific clinical signs such as decrease in avoidance movement (e.g., when approached by a handler) or decrease in appetite as indicators of pain in these animals. Research has shown that amphibians are able and motivated to learn to avoid noxious stimuli (Strickler-Shaw and Taylor 1991).

CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS

Further studies to develop robust, reliable, broadly applicable pain assessment tools are required. The general assumption is that the magnitude of the clinical signs and behavioral changes observed correlates closely with the intensity of pain. The extent to which these behavior-based assessments reflect the affective component of pain is uncertain and requires an improved understanding of the nature of pain, consciousness, and affective state in animals (see Box 1-2 in Chapter 1). Further, the lack of overlap between the assessment techniques used by veterinarians, pain researchers (Appendix A), and psychologists (Box 1-4) is an impediment to progress toward a broadly shared understanding.

The committee offers the following conclusions and recommendations:

  1. Pain in animals is difficult to assess and greatly depends on the combination of a structured clinical examination and good knowledge of the normal appearance and behavior of the animals involved.
  2. Observing animals’ response to analgesic treatment can help refine clinical assessment schemes.
  3. As more objective pain assessment schemes are developed, they should be adopted. The paucity of information for species other than farm animals, rats, and mice is detrimental to the animals’ welfare and well-being as well as the quality of scientific research.
  4. Responses of animals in analgesic drug tests and in models of pain can be used in efforts to identify (1) specific behaviors for use in assessment schemes and (2) sources of variation and factors that may influence pain intensity and analgesic efficacy.

REFERENCES

  1. Arras M, Rettich A, Cinelli P, Kasermann HP, Burki K. Assessment of post-laparotomy pain in laboratory mice by telemetric recording of heart rate and heart rate variability. BMC Vet Res Aug. 2007;2:3–16. [PMC free article: PMC1965463] [PubMed: 17683523]
  2. Ashley FH, Waterman-Pearson AE, Whay HR. Behavioural assessment of pain in horses and donkeys: Application to clinical practice and future studies. Equine Vet J. 2005;37(6):565–575. [PubMed: 16295937]
  3. Ashley PJ, Sneddon LU, McCrohan CR. Properties of corneal receptors in a teleost fish. Neurosci Lett. 2006;410(3):165–168. [PubMed: 17101221]
  4. Ashley PJ, Sneddon LU, McCrohan CR. Nociception in fish: Stimulus-response properties of receptors on the head of trout Oncorhynchus mykiss. Brain Res. 2007;1166:47–54. [PubMed: 17673186]
  5. Ashley PJ, Ringrose S, Edwards KL, Wallington E, McCrohan CR, Sneddon LU. Effect of noxious stimulation upon anti-predator responses and dominance status in rainbow trout. Anim Behav. 2009;77(2):403–410.
  6. Beukema JJ. Acquired hook-avoidance in the pike Esox lucius L. fished with artificial and natural baits. J Fish Biol. 1970a;2:155–160.
  7. Beukema JJ. Angling experiments with carp: Decreased catchability through one trial learning. Neth J Zool. 1970b;20:81–92.
  8. Beynen AC, Baumans V, Bertens AP, Havenaar R, Hesp AP, Van Zutphen LF. Assessment of discomfort in gallstone-bearing mice: A practical example of the problems encountered in an attempt to recognize discomfort in laboratory animals. Lab Anim. 1987;21(1):35–42. [PubMed: 3560862]
  9. Brodbelt DC, Taylor PM, Stanway GW. A comparison of preoperative morphine and buprenorphine for postoperative analgesia for arthrotomy in dogs. J Vet Pharmacol Ther. 1997;20(4):284–289. [PubMed: 9280368]
  10. Cambridge AJ, Tobias KM, Newberry RC, Sarkar DK. Subjective and objective measurements of postoperative pain in cats. JAVMA. 2000;217(5):685–690. [PubMed: 10976300]
  11. Clark MD, Krugner-Higby L, Smith LJ, Heath TD, Clark KL, Olson D. Evaluation of liposome-encapsulated oxymorphone hydrochloride in mice after splenectomy. Comp Med. 2004;54(5):558–563. [PubMed: 15575370]
  12. Coggeshall RE. Fos, nociception and the dorsal horn. Prog Neurobiol. 2005;77(5):299–352. [PubMed: 16356622]
  13. Cowan A, Doxey JC, Harry EJ. The animal pharmacology of buprenorphine, an oripavine analgesic agent. Br J Pharmacol. 1977;60(4):547–554. [PMC free article: PMC1667382] [PubMed: 409449]
  14. Dixon MJ, Robertson SA, Taylor PM. A thermal threshold testing device for evaluation of analgesics in cats. Res Vet Sci. 2002;72:205–210. [PubMed: 12076115]
  15. Driessen B, Zarucco L. Pain: From diagnosis to effective treatment. Clin Tech Equine P. 2007;6(2):126–134.
  16. Duncan IJ, Beatty ER, Hocking PM, Duff SR. Assessment of pain associated with degenerative hip disorders in adult male turkeys. Res Vet Sci. 1991;50(2):200–203. [PubMed: 2034900]
  17. Ehrensing RH, Michell GF, Kastin AJ. Similar antagonism of morphine analgesia by MIF-1 and naloxone in Carassius auratus. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1982;17(4):757–761. [PubMed: 6129644]
  18. Firth AM, Haldane SL. Development of a scale to evaluate postoperative pain in dogs. JAVMA. 1999;214(5):651–659. [PubMed: 10088012]
  19. Flecknell PA. Relief of pain in laboratory animals. Lab Anim. 1984;18:147–160. [PubMed: 6146743]
  20. Flecknell PA, Liles JH. The effects of surgical procedures, halothane anaesthesia and nalbuphine on locomotor activity and food and water consumption in rats. Lab Anim. 1991;25(1):50–60. [PubMed: 2010976]
  21. Gabriel AF, Marcus MA, Honig WM, Walenkamp GH, Joosten EA. The CatWalk method: A detailed analysis of behavioral changes after acute inflammatory pain in the rat. J Neurosci Methods. 2007;163(1):9–16. [PubMed: 17382401]
  22. Gaynor JS, Muir W. Handbook of Veterinary Pain Management. St. Louis, MO: Mosby; 2002.
  23. Giamberardino MA, Valente R, de Bigontina P, Vecchiet L. Artificial ureteral calculosis in rats: Behavioural characterization of visceral pain episodes and their relationship with referred lumbar muscle hyperalgesia. Pain. 1995;61(3):459–469. [PubMed: 7478690]
  24. Gonzalez MI, Field MJ, Bramwell S, McCleary S, Singh L. Ovariohysterectomy in the rat: A model of surgical pain for evaluation of pre-emptive analgesia? Pain. 2000;88(1):79–88. [PubMed: 11098102]
  25. Grisneaux E, Pibarot P, Dupuis J, Blais D. Comparison of ketoprofen and carprofen administered prior to orthopedic surgery for control of postoperative pain in dogs. JAVMA. 1999;215(8):1105–1110. [PubMed: 10530322]
  26. Hadfield CA, Whitaker BR. Amphibian emergency medicine and care. Semin Avian Exot Pet. 2005;14(2 Spec Iss):79–89.
  27. Harvey-Clark CJ, Gilespie K, Riggs KW. Transdermal fentanyl compared with parenteral buprenorphine in post-surgical pain in swine: A case study. Lab Anim. 2000;34(4):386–398. [PubMed: 11072859]
  28. Hawkins P. Recognizing and assessing pain, suffering and distress in laboratory animals: A survey of current practice in the UK with recommendations. Lab Anim. 2002;36(4):378–395. [PubMed: 12396281]
  29. Hay M, Vulin A, Génin S, Sales P, Prunier A. Assessment of pain induced by castration in piglets: Behavioral and physiological responses over the subsequent 5 days. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2003;82(3):201–218.
  30. Hayes KE, Raucci JA Jr, Gades NM, Toth LA. An evaluation of analgesic regimens for abdominal surgery in mice. Contemp Top Lab Anim. 2000;39(6):18–23. [PubMed: 11487247]
  31. Hazewinkel HAW, van den Brom WE, Theyse LFH, Pollmeier M, Hanson PD. Comparison of the effects of firocoxib, carprofen and vedaprofen in a sodium urate crystal induced synovitis model of arthritis in dogs. Res Vet Sci. 2008;84(1):74–79. [PubMed: 17408711]
  32. Hernandez J, Shearer JK, Webb DW. Effect of lameness on milk yield in dairy cows. JAVMA. 2002;220(5):640–644. [PubMed: 12418524]
  33. Hernandez JA, Garbarino EJ, Shearer JK, Risco CA, Thatcher WW. Comparison of milk yield in dairy cows with different degrees of lameness. JAVMA. 2005;227(8):1292–1296. [PubMed: 16266019]
  34. Hess A, Sergejeva M, Budinsky L, Zeilhofer HU, Brune K. Imaging of hyperalgesia in rats by functional MRI. Eur J Pain. 2007;11(1):109–119. [PubMed: 16517192]
  35. Hogan Q. Animal pain models. Reg Anesth Pain Med. 2002;27(4):385–401. [PubMed: 12132063]
  36. Holton L, Reid J, Scott EM, Pawson P, Nolan A. Development of a behaviour-based scale to measure acute pain in dogs. Vet Rec. 2001;148(17):525–531. [PubMed: 11354645]
  37. Holton LL, Scott EM, Nolan AM, Reid J, Welsh E. Relationship between physiological factors and clinical pain in dogs scored using a numerical rating scale. J Small Anim Pract. 1998;39(10):469–474. [PubMed: 9816569]
  38. Hudson C, Whay H, Hixley J. Recognition and management of pain in cattle. In Practice. 2008;30:126–134.
  39. ILAR. Pain and Distress in Fish. ILAR J. 2009;50:327–418. [PubMed: 19949248]
  40. Jansen GA, Greene NM. Morphine metabolism and morphine tolerance in goldfish. Anesthesiology. 1970;32(3):231–235. [PubMed: 5412881]
  41. Karas AZ. Postoperative analgesia in the laboratory mouse, Mus musculus. Lab Anim (NY). 2002;31(7):49–52. [PubMed: 12200594]
  42. KuKanich B, Lascelles BD, Papich MG. Assessment of a von Frey device for evaluation of the antinociceptive effects of morphine and its application in pharmacodynamic modeling of morphine in dogs. Am J Vet Res. 2005;66:1616–1622. [PubMed: 16261837]
  43. Lascelles BD, Capner C, Waterman AF. Survey of perioperative analgesic use in small animals. Vet Rec. 1995;137(26):676. [PubMed: 8966982]
  44. Lascelles BD, Cripps PJ, Jones A, Waterman AE. Post-operative central hypersensitivity and pain: The pre-emptive value of pethidine for ovariohysterectomy. Pain. 1997;73(3):461–471. [PubMed: 9469538]
  45. Le Bars D, Gozariu M, Cadden SW. Animal models of nociception. Pharmacol Rev. 2001;53(4):597–652. [PubMed: 11734620]
  46. Leach MC, Allweiler S, Richardson C, Roughan JV, Flecknell PA. Behavioural effects of ovarohysterectomy and oral administration of meloxicam in laboratory housed rabbits. Res Vet Sci. 2009;87(2):336–347. [PubMed: 19303122]
  47. Lester SJ, Mellor DJ, Ward RN, Holmes RJ. Cortisol responses of young lambs to castration and tailing using different methods. New Zeal Vet J. 1991;39(4):134–138. [PubMed: 16031640]
  48. Lester SJ, Mellor DJ, Holmes RJ, Ward RN, Stafford KJ. Behavioural and cortisol responses of lambs to castration and tailing using different methods. New Zeal Vet J. 1996;44:45–54. [PubMed: 16031893]
  49. Ley SJ, Waterman AE, Livingston A. Measurement of mechanical thresholds, plasma cortisol and catecholamines in control and lame cattle: A preliminary study. Res Vet Sci. 1996;61:172–173. [PubMed: 8880991]
  50. Liles JH, Flecknell PA. The effects of buprenorphine, nalbuphine and butorphanol alone or following halothane anaesthesia on food and water consumption and locomotor movement in rats. Lab Anim. 1992;26(3):180–189. [PubMed: 1501431]
  51. Liles JH, Flecknell PA. The effects of surgical stimulus on the rat and the influence of analgesic treatment. Br Vet J. 1993a;149(6):515–525. [PubMed: 8111612]
  52. Liles JH, Flecknell PA. The influence of buprenorphine or bupivacaine on the postoperative effects of laparotomy and bile-duct ligation in rats. Lab Anim. 1993b;27(4):374–380. [PubMed: 8277712]
  53. Lykkegaard K, Lauritzen B, Tessem L, Weikop P, Svendsen O. Local anaesthetics attenuates spinal nociception and HPA-axis activation during experimental laparotomy in pigs. Res Vet Sci. 2005;79(3):245–251. [PubMed: 16054895]
  54. Malavasi LM, Nyman G, Augustsson H, Jacobson M, Jensen-Waern M. Effects of epidural morphine and transdermal fentanyl analgesia on physiology and behaviour after abdominal surgery in pigs. Lab Anim. 2006;40(1):16–27. [PubMed: 16460586]
  55. Mathews KA, Pettifer G, Foster R, McDonell W. Safety and efficacy of preoperative administration of meloxicam, compared with that of ketoprofen and butorphanol in dogs undergoing abdominal surgery. Am J Vet Res. 2001;62(6):882–888. [PubMed: 11400845]
  56. Mellor DJ, Stafford KJ. Acute castration and/or tailing distress and its alleviation in lambs. New Zeal Vet J. 2000;48(2):33–43. [PubMed: 16032116]
  57. Mogil JS, Wilson SG, Bon K, Lee SE, Chung K, Raber P, Pieper JO, Hain HS, Belknap JK, Hubert L, Elmer GI, Chung JM, Devor M. Heritability of nociception I: Responses of 11 inbred mouse strains on 12 measures of nociception. Pain. 1999;80(1–2):67–82. [PubMed: 10204719]
  58. Molony V, Kent JE, McKendrick IJ. Validation of a method for assessment of an acute pain in lambs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2002;76(3):215–238.
  59. Molony V, Kent JE, Robertson IS. Assessment of acute and chronic pain after different methods of castration of calves. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1995;46:33–48.
  60. Morgan D, Cook CD, Picker MJ. Sensitivity to the discriminative stimulus and antinociceptive effects of mu opioids: Role of strain of rat, stimulus intensity, and intrinsic efficacy at the mu opioid receptor. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1999;289(2):965–975. [PubMed: 10215676]
  61. Morton DB, Griffiths PH. Guidelines on the recognition of pain, distress and discomfort in experimental animals and an hypothesis for assessment. Vet Rec. 1985;116(16):431–436. [PubMed: 3923690]
  62. Morton CM, Reid J, Scott EM, Holton LL, Nolan AM. Application of a scaling model to establish and validate an interval level pain scale for assessment of acute pain in dogs. Am J Vet Res. 2005;66(12):2154–2166. [PubMed: 16379662]
  63. Noonan GJ, Rand JS, Priest J, Ainscow J, Blackshaw JK. Behavioral observation of piglets undergoing tail docking, teeth clipping and ear notching. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1994;39:203–213.
  64. NRC (National Research Council). Recognition and Alleviation of Pain and Distress in Laboratory Animals. Washington: National Academy Press; 1992. [PubMed: 25144086]
  65. NRC. Recognition and Alleviation of Distress in Laboratory Animals. Washington: National Academies Press; 2008. [PubMed: 20669418]
  66. Overmier JB, Hollis KL. Teleostean telencephalon in learning. In: David RE, Northcutt G, editors. Fish Neurobiology Vol II: Higher Brain Areas and Functions. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan; 1983. pp. 265–284.
  67. Overmier JB, Hollis KL. Fish in the think tank: Learning, memory and integrated behavior. In: Olton D, Kesner R, editors. The Neurobiology of Comparative Cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1990. pp. 205–236.
  68. Parga ML. Assessment of the efficacy of meloxicam and development of a pain-scoring system based on behaviour in rabbits undergoing elective surgery. Proceedings of the 45th Annual BSAVA Congress Congress; 4th–7th April 2002; Birmingham, UK. 2002.
  69. Portavella M, Vargas JP, Torres B, Salas C. The effects of telencephalic pallial lesions on spatial, temporal, and emotional learning in goldfish. Brain Res Bull. 2002;57(3–4):397–399. [PubMed: 11922997]
  70. Portavella M, Torres B, Salas C. Avoidance response in goldfish: Emotional and temporal involvement of medical and lateral telencephalic pallium. J Neurosci. 2004;24(9):2335–2342. [PMC free article: PMC6730421] [PubMed: 14999085]
  71. Price J, Catriona S, Welsh EM, Waran NK. Preliminary evaluation of a behaviour-based system for assessment of post-operative pain in horses following arthroscopic surgery. Vet Anaesth Analg. 2003;30(3):124–137. [PubMed: 14498844]
  72. Puppe B, Schön PC, Tuchscherer A, Manteuffel G. Castration-induced vocalisation in domestic piglets, Sus scrofa: Complex and specific alterations of the vocal quality. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2005;95(1–2):67–78.
  73. Pypendop BH, Ilkiew JE, Robertson SA. Effect of intravenous administration of lidocaine on the thermal threshold in cats. Am J Vet Res. 2006;67:16–20. [PubMed: 16426206]
  74. Ranger M, Johnston CC, Anand KJS. Current controversies regarding pain assessment in neonates. Sem Perinatol. 2007;31(5):283–288. [PubMed: 17905182]
  75. Reilly SC, Quinn JP, Cossins AR, Sneddon LU. Behavioural analysis of a nociceptive event in fish: Comparisons between three species demonstrate specific responses. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2008;114:248–249.
  76. Rietmann TR, Stauffacher M, Bernasconi P, Auer JA, Weishaupt MA. The association between heart rate, heart rate variability, endocrine and behavioural pain measures in horses suffering from laminitis. J Vet Med A Physiol Pathol Clin Med. 2004;51(5):218–225. [PubMed: 15315700]
  77. Rose JD. The neurobehavioral nature of fishes and the question of awareness and pain. Rev Fish Sci. 2002;10:1–38.
  78. Roughan JV, Flecknell PA. Effects of surgery and analgesic administration on spontaneous behaviour in singly housed rats. Res Vet Sci. 2000;69(3):283–288. [PubMed: 11124101]
  79. Roughan JV, Flecknell PA. Behavioural effects of laparotomy and analgesic effects of ketoprofen and carprofen in rats. Pain. 2001;90(1–2):65–74. [PubMed: 11166971]
  80. Roughan JV, Flecknell PA. Buprenorphine: A reappraisal of its antinociceptive effects and therapeutic use in alleviating post-operative pain in animals. Lab Anim. 2002;36(3):322–343. [PubMed: 12144743]
  81. Roughan JV, Flecknell PA. Evaluation of a short duration behaviour-based post-operative pain scoring system in rats. Eur J Pain. 2003;7(5):397–406. [PubMed: 12935791]
  82. Roughan JV, Flecknell PA. Behaviour-based assessment of the duration of laparotomy-induced abdominal pain and the analgesic effects of carprofen and buprenorphine in rats. Behav Pharmacol. 2004;15(7):461–472. [PubMed: 15472568]
  83. Roughan JV, Flecknell PA. Training in behaviour-based post-operative pain scoring in rats—An evaluation based on improved recognition of analgesic requirements. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2006;96(3–4):327–342.
  84. Schaeffer D. Anesthesia and analgesia. In: Kohn DF, editor. Nontraditional Laboratory Animal Species in Anesthesia and Analgesia in Laboratory Animals. San Diego: Academic Press; 1997.
  85. Sharp J, Zammit T, Azar T, Lawson D. Recovery of male rats from major abdominal surgery after treatment with various analgesics. Contemp Top Lab Anim Sci. 2003;42(6):22–27. [PubMed: 14615956]
  86. Slingsby LS, Waterman-Pearson AE. Comparison of pethidine, buprenorphine and ketoprofen for postoperative analgesia after ovariohysterectomy in the cat. Vet Rec. 1998;143(7):185–189. [PubMed: 9762758]
  87. Slingsby LS, Jones A, Waterman-Pearson AE. Use of a fingermounted device to compare mechanical nociceptive thresholds in cats given pethidine or no medication after castration. Res Vet Sci. 2001;70:243–246. [PubMed: 11676621]
  88. Sneddon LU. Anatomical and electrophysiological analysis of the trigeminal nerve in a teleost fish, Oncorhynchus mykiss. Neurosci Lett. 2002;319(3):167–171. [PubMed: 11834319]
  89. Sneddon LU. The evidence for pain perception in fish: The use of morphine as an analgesic. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2003;83:153–162.
  90. Sneddon LU. Ethics and welfare: Pain perception in fish. B Eur Assoc Fish Pat. 2006;26(1):6–10.
  91. Sneddon LU, Braithwaite VA, Gentle MJ. Do fishes have nociceptors? Evidence for the evolution of a vertebrate sensory system. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2003a;270(1520):1115–1121. [PMC free article: PMC1691351] [PubMed: 12816648]
  92. Sneddon LU, Braithwaite VA, Gentle MJ. Novel object test: Examining nociception and fear in the rainbow trout. J Pain. 2003b;4(8):431–440. [PubMed: 14622663]
  93. Stafford KJ, Mellor DJ. Dehorning and disbudding distress and its alleviation in calves. Vet J. 2005;169(3):337–349. [PubMed: 15848777]
  94. Strickler-Shaw S, Taylor DH. Lead inhibits acquisition and retention learning in bullfrog tadpoles. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1991;13:167–173. [PubMed: 2046636]
  95. Svendsen O, Kok L, Lauritzan B. Nociception after intraperitoneal injection of a socium pentobarbitone formulation with and without lidocain in rats quantified by expression of neuronal c-fos in the spinal cord: A preliminary study. Lab Anim. 2007;41(2):197–203. [PubMed: 17430619]
  96. Veissier II, Rushen J, Colwell D, de Passillé AM. A laser-based method for measuring thermal nociception of cattle. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2000;66(4):289–304. [PubMed: 10700628]
  97. Viñuela-Fernández I, Jones E, Welsh EM, Fleetwood-Walker SM. Pain mechanisms and their implication for the management of pain in farm and companion animals. Vet J. 2007;174(2):227–239. [PubMed: 17553712]
  98. Weary DM, Braithwaite LA, Fraser D. Vocal response to pain in piglets. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1998;56(2–4):161–172.
  99. Welsh EM, Nolan AM. Effect of flumixin meglumine on the threshold to mechanical stimulation in healthy and lame sheep. Res Vet Sci. 1995;58:61–66. [PubMed: 7709063]
  100. Whiteside GT, Harrison J, Boulet J, Mark L, Pearson M, Gottshall S, Walker K. Pharmacological characterisation of a rat model of incisional pain. Br J Pharmacol. 2004;141(1):85–91. [PMC free article: PMC1574164] [PubMed: 14597606]
  101. Wright-Williams SL, Courade JP, Richardson CA, Roughan JV, Flecknell PA. Effects of vasectomy surgery and meloxicam treatment on faecal corticosterone levels and behaviour in two strains of laboratory mouse. Pain. 2007;130(1–2):108–118. [PubMed: 17196337]
  102. Zwakhalen SMG, Hamers JPH, Berger MPF. The psychometric quality and clinical usefulness of three pain assessment tools for elderly people with dementia. Pain. 2006;126(1–3):210–220. [PubMed: 16890355]

Footnotes

1

The committee acknowledges the publication of pertinent work on both small laboratory rodents and larger animal species. Readers who wish to delve into this topic are urged to begin with the cited references and expand their reading through them.

2

Loud and persistent vocalization is an occasional but unreliable expression of pain as it is more likely to signify alarm or anger.

Copyright © 2009, National Academy of Sciences.
Bookshelf ID: NBK32656

Views

  • PubReader
  • Print View
  • Cite this Page
  • PDF version of this title (1.0M)

Related information

  • PMC
    PubMed Central citations
  • PubMed
    Links to PubMed

Recent Activity

Your browsing activity is empty.

Activity recording is turned off.

Turn recording back on

See more...